Good genes + good schools = ?
George Francis investigates whether selective schools boost grades and improve social mobility.
Written by George Francis.
Toward the end of the Second World War, Britain engaged in a radical educational experiment. The all-party war coalition produced the 1944 Education Act creating “grammar schools,” state-run selective schools to take in all children in the top 25% of academic ability at age 11. Twenty years later, the experiment would be over. The then Labour Government demanded that grammar schools be converted into “state comprehensives” with no selective admissions. In 1970, Education Minister Margaret Thatcher declared that no more conversions were necessary but that no more grammar schools could be built either. A small number of grammar schools have survived in limbo, mainly in Northern Ireland and the Home Counties of Southern England.
The debate over grammar schools has always been divisive. Sir Anthony Crosland, who ordered their closure, once told his wife, “If it’s the last thing I do, I’m going to destroy every fucking grammar school in England.” And more recently, journalist Peter Hitchens vilified the enemies of grammar schools as “fanatical utopians.”
What makes the debate so fierce is that it strikes at the heart of a core political conflict: egalitarianism versus meritocracy—equality or equality of opportunity? In fact, the term “meritocracy” itself was created in the debate over British grammar schools. It arose as the title of Dr Michael Young’s 1958 satire attacking the grammar school system and IQ testing in general.
The idea of selective schooling in England arose from the Government’s 1924 Hadow report on “Psychological tests of educable capacity and their possible use in the public system of education.” It was written with the consultation and influence of Sir Charles Spearman and Sir Cyril Burt, two infamous scientists who respectively discovered the existence of ‘general intelligence’ and demonstrated that it was highly heritable.
The meritocratic proponents of grammar schools believe that separating children by ability allows for a more tailored education, promoting social mobility and human flourishing. By contrast, the egalitarians believe the system is unfair, providing elite schooling to those already born lucky, whether by genetics or environment, at the expense of everyone else.
That this debate provokes great fervor on both sides is understandable, but we should strive to get beyond this to examine the facts dispassionately. How much do grammar schools raise educational standards? How much do grammar schools promote social mobility? Which side of the debate is more accurate in its claims?
Let’s start with the key claim of grammar school proponents that they boost educational standards and attainment. Grammar schools do get better results compared to non-selective state schools, known as comprehensives. Grammar school grades in GCSE exams are around 1.2 standard deviations higher on average, equivalent to the scores of those in the 85th percentile. However, grammar schools also select the best students. What we want to know is whether grammar school students perform better than expected after accounting for prior abilities.
One approach is to use multivariate regression to determine whether going to grammar school predicts higher grades than what is predicted from one’s primary school grades. There are many of these analyses. One example is the 2008 report by the educational charity Sutton Trust. Their main result found that when grades in primary school (Key Stage 2) were controlled for, grammar school children scored half a grade higher at age 16 in the GCSE exam.
So should we conclude that grammar schools work? Not so fast. There is an important problem with the methodology. So long as the tests used by grammar schools provide additional information about the child’s intelligence, we would expect students who get in to outperform students who don’t with the same Key Stage 2 scores. The problem is made severe by the ‘low ceiling’ of the Key Stage 2 test. Historically, for each subject, around 25% of primary school students achieved the highest grade of 5 in Mathematics, Reading, and Writing. Many students could achieve level 5 in Key Stage 2 and would still be much less intelligent than the students who get into grammar schools.
We need to control for more and better indicators of prior ability. A recent paper did just that. It was produced by scientists at Kings College London and Toby Young, the son of Michael. They controlled for measures of prior ability, socioeconomic status, and even a genetic estimate of the student’s intelligence. They found that studying at a grammar school instead of a comprehensive resulted in a grade increase of 0.18 standard deviations, equivalent to moving from the 50th percentile to the 57th percentile.
We might still wonder whether these statistical controls are really good enough. So long as the grammar school’s tests tell them something more than family background and prior ability, we should still expect their students to perform better. To solve the issue, we need the gold standard of scientific inference — the randomized control trial.
If borderline students were randomly assigned to a comprehensive school or a grammar school, we could then know for certain if there really was an effect. And yet, despite the fiery debate over grammar schools, nobody has conducted such a rigorous experiment. The best comparisons we have are from experiments randomly giving parents vouchers for private schooling. Private schools will typically attract more able students, making them like grammar schools. Whilst these studies have shown success in the developing world, in the United States, it is estimated that going to a private school only increases grades by 0.028 standard deviations, equivalent to going from the 50th to the 51st percentile.
When the most rigorous statistical tests are used, moving into elite schooling does not seem to matter much for grades. It seems very unlikely that grammar schools, as they exist today, lead to better outcomes. This may not have always been the case. At least prior to 1964, grammar schools trained their students for a more thorough curriculum and tough examinations compared to what was taught in other government schools. However, so long as they are teaching the same curriculum, it is unlikely that the selective nature of grammar schools helps their students to learn much more.
Perhaps grammar schools provide social mobility even if they do not deliver better grades? Going to an elite school can teach you etiquette and give you a middle-upper-class social network that can help in the job market. Placing talented working-class students in this environment could help them better realize their potential.
Because of the staggered conversion of grammar schools into comprehensives across time and also across geography, some economists attempted to identify whether these conversions were associated with lower mobility or not. The authors used census data for 90,000 individuals, which record their occupation and their parent’s occupation. The stronger parental occupation predicts the occupational status of the child, the less relative social mobility. The authors then measured exposure to the grammar school system with a “selectivity index” representing the proportion of students in the local area attending the selective system.
So what did the authors find? Across time and local authorities, there was no relationship between the selectivity index and social mobility. Perhaps the social network was not so useful after all?
The meritocratic ideal of grammar schools certainly is charmingly romantic. The rags-to-riches story of working-class lads being given the opportunity to use their talents to great personal and public success is certainly desirable. But in practice, and whilst the research is far from ideal, the evidence that grammar schools are meritocratic is weak.
Perhaps, if some better studies were run, randomizing which marginal students get in, we would finally know for sure whether grammar schools matter. Then we could end the grammar school limbo, building more of them or shutting the remaining schools down. But then again, if it really were just a technical question of grades and mobility, surely we would have solved it by now?
Perhaps the grammar school debate was never about social mobility after all. Beneath the veneer of ‘meritocracy’ lies an ugly truth of the debate over grammar schools — classism. The middle class wants to avoid sending their children to school with the troublesome and dull. And those who cannot get into a grammar school hardly think it fair that their schools are not so nice. Certainly many claim to believe in ‘solidarity’ — we should mix all the classes together or at least make the rich pay to send their children to private schools. Yet when it comes to the schooling of their own children, the virtuous many become shameless elitists.
George Francis is a writer and social scientist interested in intelligence and economics. If you like deep data dives, you should check out his Substack, Anglo Reaction. Find him on Twitter here.
At one of those expensive boarding schools (I wasn't boarding -- 25k-40k/yr in past, about $37-71k now), there was little difference in marginal improvement of outcomes of the students' relative to their intellectual aptitude. The top students in Math in Math-oriented olympiads, competitions were always the NE asians. When my AP chem teacher said this is the 'best year' we had in terms of results, it was once again mostly because the class composition changed to NE asians. There were some bright European-ancestry people there, and some people from the middle east too who skipped a grade or two, took AP/IB classes.
In terms of intellectual endowment, I didn't really see any differential. Everyone self-segregated into their own racial groups at lunch, or cliques since elementary school -- with the exception of native-born second-gen+ later descendants like Asians. There were almost nil black people besides maybe a female who was fairly disruptive, a few Caribbeans. Hazing was normal, bullying (i.e. especially fat girls and short boys) was normal, those not-so-bright or academically minded took some drugs, some leftover second or third-sons being sent to a daycare centre.. basically. Certain white girls (i.e. children of teachers) were sexually promiscuous with boys sexting photos in HS. Same observation made with regards to FB photos with females in cops/maid outfits around age 12 which were shortly deleted thereafter (this one at a public immersion school though).
I noticed everyone was 30% higher in height relative to the normal population you would see in Walmart, and their muscle masses equivalents were higher as excerise/fitness/extracurriculars were a given. The most politically-minded likely majored in pol-sci, humanities, etc and took up government post positions while other kids either took a standard professional route or didn't change much relative to their parents' class (i.e. wealthy farmers, technical coding, professor).
Relative to a public school, the disruptiveness factor is probably lower and the discipline/conscientiousness factor was probably higher. Science club was 100% NE asian, aviation club was mostly white, choir 70/30 F/M ... overall whatever interests related to their groups' origins were reflected in the outcomes of their choices and the subjects they took.
I'd say the average ability was 80 percentile give or take in intellect. Before that school I was in a french-immersion program and I observed about 1 in 16 children or so had an IQ about 125+ or so given they were in a gifted program. The only point of the schools were to streamline the demographics in my mind. The so-called connections were totally dependent on the child's personality + whether they started early in elementary school together so they might have wealthy intermixing (i.e. attending each other's villas, spending a few $k on the weekends).
I did not observe any differences (that would be noticeable in regards to the 'quality' of the education or 'outcomes' in changing the students' behaviour). Except for the white boys being against our woke english teacher in grade 12. Not that woke, except of the insistence of equalization -- and him being shocked at the responses.
I've been promoting the work of Freddie deBoer whenever the topic of education comes up. Education does not "work". A lot of people, especially those on the left, think that schools make a difference. They don't. Schools are merely a way for kids to showcase their genetic propensities.
Two very good articles from Freddie on this:
https://freddiedeboer.substack.com/p/education-doesnt-work-20
https://freddiedeboer.substack.com/p/education-commentary-is-dominated